|
|
Grid illusion
A
grid illusion is any kind of grid that deceives a person's vision.
The two most common types of grid illusions are Hermann grid illusions
and Scintillating grid illusions.
Illusions such as these, and others, provide a window onto the way the
eyes and the brain work together in creating perception. Scientists
attempt to peer through this window when they propose hypotheses about
how perception is accomplished. Illusions can also help us realize that
our own perceptions may be limited or different from those of another
person viewing the same thing. |
Hermann
grid illusions
A
Hermann grid illusion. Shape position and color contrast converge
to produce the illusion of gray blobs at the intersections.
The Hermann grid illusion was first reported by Ludimar Hermann
in 1870, who discovered the illusion while reading John Tyndall's
On Sound. In 1872, Ewald Hering observed that inverse colors (a
black grid on a white background) produced similar results. Because
of this, the Hermann grid is often referred to as the "Hermann-Hering"
grid. |
Scintillating
grid illusions
An
example of the scintillating grid illusion.
Dark dots seem to appear and disappear at intersections.
The scintillating grid illusion is an optical illusion discovered
by Elke Lingelbach in 1994, and is usually considered a variation
of the Hermann grid illusion. Lingelbach and colleagues published
their findings in a 1995 article entitled "The Hermann grid
and the scintillation effect". |
|
|
|
Description
of the Hermann grid illusion
The Hermann grid illusion is created
with a grid of black squares upon a white background.
"Ghostlike" gray figures are perceived at the
intersections of the white lines. These figures disappear
when one looks directly at an intersection.
Description
of the Scintillating grid illusions
The scintillating grid illusion
is similar in construction. Instead of white bars, however,
there are gray bars with white discs inserted at each
intersection. When viewers moves their eyes around the
image, black dots seem to appear and disappear. The illusion
is enhanced by eye movement, and decreased by moving too
close or too far away from the image..
Explanation
The
effect of both optical illusions is commonly explained
by a neural process called lateral inhibition. Retinal
cells in the eye function as light receptors. If only
a single receptor is illuminated, it perceives a larger
amount of light than it does when neighboring receptors
are also illuminated. The illumination of receptors inhibits
the firing of nearby receptors, and the effect is transmitted
laterally. In the case of the Hermann grid illusion, the
setup of the white bands creates a situation where there
is more light surrounding the intersections than there
is along the bands between intersections. Thus the region
of the intersection is more inhibited, and darker spots
appear. The effect is greater when the grid is viewed
peripherally, since lateral inhibition works over greater
distances in peripheral vision.
Scientists
at MIT's Schiller Lab have disputed this widely accepted
theory, proposing an alternate type of retinal functioning
as an explanation for the illusion. Advocates of such
alternative theories argue that the Hermann grid effect
is not size dependent, works equally well with contrast
reversal, and that there are misconceptions in the function
of retinal cells that are assumed by the lateral inhibition
theory. The proposed alternate theory, called the "S1
simple-cell theory," suggests that the illusion results
from reactions within the cortex, not the retinal cells.
Both
types of grid illusions, as well as related illusions,
are excellent tools that help further the study of neuroscience.
Scientists can use anomalies like perceptual illusions
to try to understand more precisely the processes involved
in vision and perception.
New
World Encyclopedia
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
Differences
between the scintillating and Hermann grid illusions
The difference between the Hermann grid illusion and the scintillating
illusion is that scintillating illusions have dots already in place
at the intersection, whereas there are no dots already in place
at the intersections of Hermann grid illusions. Since they are so
similar, the two names are commonly used interchangeably. But the
scintillating illusion does not occur with an isolated intersection,
as in the case of the Hermann grid; observations suggest that a
minimum of 3 × 3 evenly spaced intersections with superimposed
discs are required to produce the effect. This requirement suggests
the participation of global processes of the kind proposed for the
linking and grouping of features in an image, in addition to local
processes. |
|
|
|
|
The classical explanation
Why do we see the dark patches?
Look at the left part of the left diagram and assume an on-center
retinal ganglion cell. Its receptive field is indicated by the reddish
disk. When the ganglion cell is, by chance, looking at the grating
so that its centre ('+') is positioned at a crossing (left-top),
there are 4 bright patches in the inhibitory surround. A ganglion
cell looking at a street (left-bottom) however only gets 2 inhibitory
patches, so it will have a higher spike rate then the one at the
crossings. This was measured by Baumgartner (1960) in Freiburg,
see picture on the right.
Why don't we see the patches when we look right at them?
Because then we direct the fovea at the crossings, and in the fovea
the receptive fields are much smaller (see the small reddish disks
on the right of the left figure). With such small receptive fields
it obviously does not matter whether they are at the crossings or
not. |
|
|
|
The
effect occurs when contrast is reversed |
|
Color
of smudges is defined by which set of lines is in front. On the left color
lines are in front and smudges have the same color.
On the right the gray lines are in front and the smudges are darker gray.
|
|
|
Hermann
Grid, curving
When the grid lines are straight, dark patches appear
in the street crossings, except the ones which you are directly looking
at.
When the streets are curving, the dark patches vanish.
|
|
|
"Lighting
grid illusion" Illusory
colored rays appear to run obliquely
Copyright Keizo Shimizu 2004 |
|
|
Optical
Illusions REFERENCES:
• "Hermann Grid" 1997. Illusion Works. Retrieved October
30, 2007.
• "The Neural Control of Vision" MIT. Retrieved October
30, 2007.
• "The Neural Control of Vision" MIT. Retrieved October
30, 2007.
• Hermann L (1870). "Eine Erscheinung simultanen Contrastes.".
Pflügers Archiv für die gesamte Physiologie. 3: 13–15.
• Alexander, D.M.; Van Leeuwen, C. (2010). "Mapping of
contextual modulation in the population response of primary visual
cortex".
• Baumgartner G (1960). "Indirekte Größenbestimmung
der rezeptiven Felder der Retina beim Menschen mittels der Hermannschen
Gittertäuschung."
• Geier J, Bernáth L, Hudák M, Séra L (2008).
"Straightness as the main factor of the Hermann grid illusion".
Perception. 37 (5): 651–665.
• Geier, János (2008). "Stopping the Hermann grid
illusion by sine distortion".
• Schiller, P. H., & Carvey, C. E. (2005). The Hermann grid
illusion revisited. Perception-London, 34(11), 1375-1398.
• Schiller PH, Carvey CE (2005). "The Hermann grid illusion
revisited.". Perception. 34 (11): 1375–97. doi:10.1068/p5447.
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
Copyright
© 2004 ABC-people.com
Design and conception BeStudio © 2016-2018 |
|
|
|
|